Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Educational differences in the impact of pictorial cigarette warning labels on smokers: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Europe surveys
  1. Gera E Nagelhout1,2,
  2. Marc C Willemsen1,2,
  3. Hein de Vries1,
  4. Ute Mons3,4,
  5. Sara C Hitchman5,6,
  6. Anton E Kunst7,
  7. Romain Guignard8,
  8. Mohammad Siahpush9,
  9. Hua-Hie Yong10,
  10. Bas van den Putte11,12,
  11. Geoffrey T Fong13,14,
  12. James F Thrasher15,16
  1. 1Department of Health Promotion, Maastricht University (CAPHRI), Maastricht, The Netherlands
  2. 2Dutch Alliance for a Smokefree Society, The Hague, The Netherlands
  3. 3Division of Clinical Epidemiology and Aging Research, German Cancer Research Center (DKFZ), Heidelberg, Germany
  4. 4Unit Cancer Prevention, German Cancer Research Center (DKFZ), Heidelberg, Germany
  5. 5UK Centre for Tobacco and Alcohol Studies (UKCTAS), London, UK
  6. 6Department of Addictions, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King's College London, London, UK
  7. 7Department of Public Health, Academic Medical Centre, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
  8. 8French Institute for Health Promotion and Health Education (INPES), Saint-Denis, France
  9. 9Department of Health Promotion, Social and Behavioral Health, College of Public Health, University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, Nebraska, USA
  10. 10VicHealth Centre for Tobacco Control, The Cancer Council Victoria, Carlton, Victoria, Australia
  11. 11Department of Communication, University of Amsterdam (ASCoR), Amsterdam, The Netherlands
  12. 12Trimbos Institute, Netherlands Institute for Mental Health and Addiction, Utrecht, The Netherlands
  13. 13Department of Psychology, School of Public Health and Health Systems, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada
  14. 14Ontario Institute for Cancer Research, Toronto, Ontario, Canada
  15. 15Department of Health Promotion, Education and Behaviour, University of South Carolina, Columbia, South Carolina, USA
  16. 16Department of Tobacco Research, Mexican National Institute of Public Health, Cuernavaca, Mexico
  1. Correspondence to Dr Gera E Nagelhout, Department of Health Promotion, Maastricht University (CAPHRI), P.O. Box 616, Maastricht 6200 MD, The Netherlands; Gera.Nagelhout{at}maastrichtuniversity.nl

Abstract

Objective To examine (1) the impact of pictorial cigarette warning labels on changes in self-reported warning label responses: warning salience, cognitive responses, forgoing cigarettes and avoiding warnings, and (2) whether these changes differed by smokers’ educational level.

Methods Longitudinal data of smokers from two survey waves of the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Europe Surveys were used. In France and the UK, pictorial warning labels were implemented on the back of cigarette packages between the two survey waves. In Germany and the Netherlands, the text warning labels did not change.

Findings Warning salience decreased between the surveys in France (OR=0.81, p=0.046) and showed a non-significant increase in the UK (OR=1.30, p=0.058), cognitive responses increased in the UK (OR=1.34, p<0.001) and decreased in France (OR=0.70, p=0.002), forgoing cigarettes increased in the UK (OR=1.65, p<0.001) and decreased in France (OR=0.83, p=0.047), and avoiding warnings increased in France (OR=2.93, p<0.001) and the UK (OR=2.19, p<0.001). Warning salience and cognitive responses decreased in Germany and the Netherlands, forgoing did not change in these countries and avoidance increased in Germany. In general, these changes in warning label responses did not differ by education. However, in the UK, avoidance increased especially among low (OR=2.25, p=0.001) and moderate educated smokers (OR=3.21, p<0.001).

Conclusions The warning labels implemented in France in 2010 and in the UK in 2008 with pictures on one side of the cigarette package did not succeed in increasing warning salience, but did increase avoidance. The labels did not increase educational inequalities among continuing smokers.

  • Socioeconomic status
  • Packaging and Labelling
  • Public policy

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

In most Western countries, tobacco smoking is more prevalent among people with low compared to high educational levels.1–3 To help mitigate resulting health disparities, it is important that there are tobacco control policies that decrease educational differences in smoking. To date, however, there has been too little attention paid to the question of whether the impact of tobacco control policies varies across educational levels.4 This paper examined whether the impact of pictorial warning labels on cigarette packages implemented in France and the UK varied across educational levels of smokers. Educational differences in changes in four self-reported warning label responses were studied: warning salience, cognitive responses, forgoing cigarettes and avoiding warnings.

Studies using population-based surveys found that smokers from high educated groups reported noticing text-only warning labels (warning salience) more often than smokers from low educated groups.5 ,6 Additionally, smokers from high educated groups reported higher perceived effectiveness of the text-only warning labels increasing their thoughts about health risks of smoking and about quitting (cognitive responses).7

For pictorial warning labels, experimental and population-based survey studies both found equal effectiveness for low and high educated groups on warning salience6 ,8 and cognitive responses.8–11 Three studies on pictorial warning labels in Mexico and Brazil found that lower educated groups had stronger cognitive responses,6 ,12 ,13 and more often reported that warning labels stopped them from having a cigarette (forgoing cigarettes).6

There is evidence that the implementation of pictorial warning labels is followed by an increase in the number of smokers covering up the warnings, keeping them out of sight or using a cigarette case (avoiding warnings).14–16 To the best of our knowledge, no studies have examined educational differences in (changes in) avoidance of cigarette warning labels.

A study about educational differences in warning label impact in Europe showed mixed results. This study examined self-reported responses to text-only warning labels with cross-sectional population-based survey data from France, Germany, the Netherlands and the UK. Scores on a combined measure of salience, cognitive responses and forgoing cigarettes due to the warning labels were higher among low to moderate educated French, German and Dutch smokers than among high educated smokers, while the opposite trend was observed in the UK.17 Differences in avoidance of warning labels were not reported. Changes in educational differences after the impact of the European pictorial warning labels have not been studied yet.

Pictorial cigarette warning labels are not yet mandatory in the European Union. The European Commission directive 2001/37/EC came into effect in 2003 and required cigarettes sold in the European Union to carry text warning labels. Two general warnings (‘smoking kills’ and ‘smoking seriously harms you and others around you’) must be placed on 30% of the front of the cigarette pack and one of a rotating set of 14 additional text warnings must be placed on 40% of the back of the pack. Optional colour photographs and other illustrations for the back of the pack were adopted by the European Commission in 2005. Member states can choose between 3 illustrations for each of the 14 additional warnings, some showing pictures of diseased organs and human suffering and others showing messages to encourage smokers to get help with quitting. The UK implemented pictorial warnings in 2008 and France in 2010 (table 1). Although they are called pictorial warning labels, not all of them contain photographs. The UK chose 11 labels with photographs and 4 labels with text-based images, while France adopted 14 labels with photographs and no text-based images (figure 1). In France and the UK, factory-made cigarette packages were first obligated to carry pictorial warning labels, while roll-your-own tobacco had a longer phase-in period (table 1).

Table 1

Warning label policies, fieldwork periods, sample sizes and follow-up rates per country

Figure 1

Examples of the optional European pictorial warning labels for the back of cigarette packages. Pictures (A) and (F) are used in the UK, pictures (B) and (E) are used in France, and pictures (C) and (D) are used in the UK and France.

In the current study, we examined the impact of pictorial warning labels on changes in self-reported warning label responses among smokers with low, moderate and high levels of education by using a quasi-experimental design. We used longitudinal data from two survey waves of the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Europe Surveys conducted in France, the UK, Germany and the Netherlands. In France and the UK (‘intervention’ countries), pictorial warning labels were implemented between the two survey waves. In Germany and the Netherlands (‘control’ countries), the text warning labels did not change. Our research questions were:

  1. Was the implementation of pictorial warning labels in France and the UK associated with a change in self-reported warning label responses that was larger than any change in these responses in Germany and the Netherlands, where no pictorial warning labels were implemented?

  2. Were these changes in self-reported warning label responses between baseline and follow-up different for low, moderate and high educated smokers?

Methods

Sample

Longitudinal survey data from smokers aged 18 years and older from the ITC Europe Surveys in France, the UK, Germany and the Netherlands were used. The mode of interviewing was computer-assisted telephone interviewing in France, the UK and Germany, and computer-assisted web interviewing in the Netherlands. Telephone respondents were recruited using probability sampling methods with fixed line telephone numbers selected at random from the population of each country. Web respondents were recruited from a large probability-based database with respondents who had indicated their willingness to participate in research on a regular basis.18 Respondents in all countries were eligible to participate at initial recruitment into the survey if they had smoked at least 100 cigarettes in their lifetime and were currently smoking at least once a month.

For France, we used one survey wave from before the implementation of pictorial warning labels (wave 2, 2008) and one survey wave after the implementation (wave 3, 2012). For the UK, we also used one survey wave before (wave 6, 2007–2008) and one after (wave 8, 2010) the implementation of pictorial warning labels. For Germany and the Netherlands, two survey waves were used when text warning labels were on cigarette packages: one survey wave from 2007 or 2008 and one survey wave from 2011. In all countries, we limited our analyses to current smokers because frequency of exposure to warning labels, and cognitive and behavioural responses to warning labels, is reduced once people quit smoking and not all questions were asked of ex-smokers in all countries.

Across countries, 6366 current smokers participated in the baseline surveys that were used in this study. Of them, 2863 respondents participated in the follow-up survey and were still smoking at follow-up (table 1). These smokers differed from baseline smokers who were lost to follow-up or who stopped smoking: continuing smokers who participated in the follow-up survey were more often female in the Netherlands (χ2=4.23, p=0.040), were older in all countries (F France=11.12, p<0.001; UK=13.36, p<0.001; Germany=18.63, p<0.001; the Netherlands=36.70, p<0.001), had lower education in the Netherlands (χ2=13.26, p=0.001), were more often exclusively smoking rolling tobacco in the Netherlands (χ2=7.62, p=0.006), were heavier smokers in all countries except France (F UK=7.48, p=0.006; Germany=4.26, p=0.039; the Netherlands=15.00, p<0.001) and were less often planning to quit smoking within 6 months in all countries (χ2 France=15.03, p<0.001; UK=6.85, p=0.009; Germany=4.36, p=0.037; the Netherlands=15.22, p<0.001). Respondents who participated in the follow-up survey and were still smoking did not differ significantly from smokers who were lost to follow-up or who stopped smoking, on warning salience, cognitive responses, forgoing cigarettes and avoiding warnings at baseline in any of the countries.

Measurements

Education was categorised into three levels: low (no degree, elementary school and lower secondary education), moderate (secondary vocational education and middle secondary education) and high (upper secondary education, university and postgraduation). The education levels were only partly comparable across countries because of differences in educational systems.

Warning salience was measured with the question “In the last month, how often have you noticed the warning labels on cigarette packages or on roll-your-own packs?”14 Responses were dichotomised into ever (1) versus never (0).

Cognitive responses to warning labels were measured with three questions: “To what extent, if at all, do the warning labels make you think about the health risks of smoking?”, “To what extent, if at all, do the warning labels on cigarette packs make you more likely to quit smoking?” and “In the past 6 months, have warning labels on cigarette packages led you to think about quitting?”.14 All responses were dichotomised into ever (1) versus never (0) and combined by computing a binary variable: any cognitive response (1) versus no cognitive responses (0).

Forgoing cigarettes because of warning labels was measured by asking respondents “In the last month, have the warning labels stopped you from having a cigarette when you were about to smoke one?”14 Responses to this question were dichotomised into ever (1) versus never (0).

Avoidance of warning labels was measured in the UK at both survey waves with the question “In the last month, have you made any effort to avoid looking at or thinking about the warning labels, such as covering them up, keeping them out of sight, using a cigarette case, avoiding certain warnings, or any other means?”14 In the Netherlands, a shortened version of the question was used at both survey waves: “In the last month, have you made any effort to avoid looking at or thinking about the warning labels by covering the warnings up?” France and Germany used the shortened version of the question at baseline and the longer version at follow-up. Respondents could answer these questions with (1) yes or (0) no.

Control variables were gender, age group (18–24, 25–39, 40–54, 55+), tobacco type (smoking exclusively rolling tobacco or (also) manufactured cigarettes), Heaviness of Smoking Index (HSI), intention to quit, country and time in sample (number of prior survey waves in which the person had participated). HSI was computed by taking the sum of two categorised measures: number of cigarettes per day and time to the first cigarette of the day.19 HSI values ranged from 0 to 6 with higher values indicating stronger nicotine dependence.19

Analyses

All analyses were performed with SPSS V.22.0 and were weighted by age and gender to be representative of the smoker population in each country. Generalised Estimating Equations (GEE) analyses were performed to examine country, time and educational level differences in (1) warning salience, (2) cognitive responses, (3) forgoing cigarettes and (4) avoiding warnings. The data from all countries were pooled for the primary analyses. All dependent variables were dichotomous and, therefore, the binomial distribution and the logit link were used.20 The repeated measure variable was survey wave: baseline or follow-up. The unstructured correlation structure was used, because this closely approximates the true correlation structure in large samples.21 All GEE models were adjusted for time-invariant covariates (gender, age group at baseline and country) and time-varying covariates (educational level, tobacco type, HSI, intention to quit and time in sample).

In separate GEE analyses, we added interactions between country and survey wave (research question 1), and between country, survey wave and educational level (research question 2). We adjusted these analyses for the interaction between educational level and survey wave, and the interaction between educational level and country. Dummy coding was used for educational level, country and survey wave in the main and interaction analyses.22 The p values for the overall 3 degrees of freedom (df) tests for the country by wave interaction and the 6df tests for the education by country by wave interaction were reported in the table and text. When the overall tests of interactions were significant, single degree of freedom interactions were also examined, but these test statistics are not reported in the tables or the text. To facilitate interpretations for readers, table 2 also shows stratified analyses separately for each country.

Table 2

GEE analyses† predicting warning label responses, pooled across countries with interactions and separately for each country

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 3 shows the sample characteristics for the smoking respondents at the baseline surveys in France, UK, Germany and the Netherlands. Respondents from the four countries significantly differed on gender, age group, educational level, tobacco type, HSI and intention to quit smoking.

Table 3

Sample characteristics at the baseline survey and time in sample at both surveys by country (weighted data)

Warning salience

The percentage of respondents who noticed warning labels in the past month by country, survey wave and educational level can be seen in table 4. Between 74% (Germany, follow-up survey) and 91% (UK, follow-up survey) of smokers noticed the warning labels in the past month.

Table 4

Warning label responses by country, survey wave, and educational level (weighted data)

The GEE analysis in table 2 indicated that warning salience decreased between baseline and follow-up surveys (OR=0.72, p<0.001). In an additional GEE analysis, we added interactions between country, survey wave and educational level (table 2). The interaction between country and wave was significant (p<0.001). Stratified analyses showed that warning salience decreased in France (OR=0.81, p=0.046), showed a non-significant increase in the UK (OR=1.30, p=0.058), and decreased in Germany (OR=0.62, p<0.001) and the Netherlands (OR=0.47, p<0.001).

The three-way interaction between educational level, country and wave was not significant (p=0.641).

Cognitive responses

Between 42% (the Netherlands, follow-up survey) and 90% (France, baseline survey) of smokers reported thoughts about health risks or quitting due to warning labels (table 4).

Table 2 shows the GEE analysis predicting cognitive responses. Cognitive responses decreased between the two survey waves (OR=0.79, p<0.001). An additional GEE analysis with interactions (table 2) showed that the interaction between country and wave was significant (p<0.001). Cognitive responses to warning labels increased in the UK (OR=1.34, p<0.001), but decreased in the other countries (OR France=0.70, p=0.002; Germany=0.83, p=0.037; the Netherlands=0.51, p<0.001).

The three-way interaction between educational level, country and wave was not significant (p=0.248).

In additional analyses (data not shown), the three survey questions about cognitive responses were analysed separately. Affirmative responses to the question whether the warning labels made smokers think about quitting increased between baseline and follow-up for all countries combined, and there was no significant interaction between country and wave. For the other two questions (whether the warning labels made smokers think about the health risks of smoking and made them more likely to quit) the interaction between country and wave was significant. These cognitive responses increased in the UK and decreased in the other countries.

Forgoing cigarettes

Between 4% (Germany, follow-up survey) and 23% (France, baseline survey) of smokers reported that warning labels stopped them from having a cigarette in the past month (table 4).

As can be seen in table 2, forgoing did not change between survey waves (OR=1.00, p=0.938). In the GEE analysis with interactions we found a significant interaction between country and wave (p=0.001). Stratified analyses showed that reports of forgoing cigarettes because of warning labels increased in the UK (OR=1.65, p<0.001), decreased in France (OR=0.83, p=0.047) and did not change significantly in the other countries (OR Germany=0.71, p=0.135; the Netherlands=1.06, p=0.742).

The three-way interaction between educational level, country and wave was not significant (p=0.122).

Avoiding warnings

Between 2% (Germany, baseline survey) and 18% (the UK, follow-up survey) of smokers reported that they had made an effort to avoid looking at or thinking about the warning labels in the past month (table 4).

Table 2 shows the GEE analysis for avoidance. Avoidance increased between baseline and follow-up surveys (OR=2.31, p<0.001). A separate GEE analysis (table 2), showed a significant interaction between country and survey wave (p=0.049). Country-stratified analyses indicated that avoidance increased between baseline and follow-up in France (OR=2.93, p<0.001), the UK (OR=2.19, p<0.001) and Germany (OR=2.57, p<0.001), but not significantly so in the Netherlands (OR=1.28, p=0.304).

As can be seen in table 2, the three-way interaction between educational level, country and wave was significant (p=0.002). Analyses stratified by country (not shown in Tables) indicated that in the UK, the increase in avoidance between baseline and follow-up was especially pronounced among low (OR=2.25, p<0.001) and moderate educated smokers (OR=3.21, p<0.001). In the Netherlands, while there was no increase in avoidance among low and moderate educated smokers, there was a significant increase among high educated smokers (OR=3.52, p=0.036).

Discussion

The current study showed that after the implementation of pictorial warning labels in France and the UK, warning salience did not increase, cognitive responses and forgoing of cigarettes increased only in the UK, and avoidance of warnings increased substantially in France and the UK. This is contrary to what has been found in previous pre–post studies, in which marked increases in all four warning label responses were observed after the implementation of pictorial warning labels.14–16 ,23 A possible explanation is that the current European pictorial warning labels are placed on only one side of the cigarette package, while other countries have placed them at the back as well as at the front of the package. Previous studies6 ,24 already found indications that it is important to place pictorial warnings on both the front and back of the package. Our study is the first to have examined the impact of pictorial warnings on one side of the package with a pre–post population-based survey study.

The other research question that we examined was whether there were educational differences in the change in warning label responses over time. Few studies have examined whether the impact of warning labels varies across educational levels.4 Studies that have examined educational differences have not always used nationally representative samples surveyed both before and after the implementation of new warning labels. We found a significant difference in the increase in avoidance in the UK compared to the Netherlands. It seems that the increase in avoidance of warning labels between waves was larger among smokers with low and moderate levels of education compared to more highly educated smokers in the UK. In the Netherlands, there was no increase in avoidance among low and moderate educated smokers, but there was a significant increase among high educated smokers. Other educational differences in trends of self-reported warning label responses across countries were not significant. Therefore, we conclude that our findings point to a neutral equity impact of pictorial warning labels among continuing smokers. It should be noted that the results could have been different if we had also examined those who quit smoking.

The increase in cognitive responses and forgoing cigarettes only in the UK and not in France can possibly be explained by the set of pictorial warnings that the UK chose. The UK chose 4 out of 15 labels with text-based images, while France adopted 14 labels with photographs and no labels with text-based images (figure 1). The UK set may have been more effective in stimulating thoughts about health risks of smoking and thoughts about quitting, and more effective in stopping smokers from having a cigarette than the French set. However, it is also possible that the longer period between the implementation of pictorial warnings and the follow-up survey in France (table 1) played a role. It should be noted that additional analyses showed that one of the cognitive responses (warning labels made smokers think about quitting) did increase in France.

In Germany and the Netherlands, no pictorial warning labels were implemented and the text labels have been the same since 2003 and 2002, respectively. We found that warning salience and cognitive responses decreased in these countries. This pattern of decreases of self-reported warning label responses (warning label ‘wear-out’) has been reported before.14 ,15 ,25 We found that warning avoidance increased in Germany, but avoidance remained much lower (6% tried to avoid labels in the past month at the second survey wave) than in France (17%) and the UK (18%) after the implementation of pictorial warning labels. The small increase in avoidance in Germany and (part of) the increase in France could have been due to the change in measurement of avoidance between the baseline and follow-up surveys in these countries.

Limitations

This study's limitations are, first of all, that there were 4 years between the baseline and follow-up survey in France. Note that this does not mean that we could only assess the long-term impact of the implementation of pictorial warnings, because the follow-up surveys in France and the UK were conducted only a few months after the pictorial warnings were fully implemented (this took 2 years in both countries, see table 1). Therefore, we could still show the immediate impact of the full implementation of pictorial warnings in both countries. In the UK, Germany and the Netherlands, because we chose survey waves that approximately matched the timing of 4 years between the two surveys studied in France, these countries had one or more survey waves completed in between the two studied waves. Any effect of this on our results is likely to be small as all analyses were controlled for time in sample. However, there could have been effects from other tobacco control activity that has happened between the baseline and follow-up surveys. For example, the price of cigarettes increased in France and there were several media campaigns about smoking. In the UK, there were no substantial price increases, but there were media campaigns about smoking during the study period. Germany and the Netherlands both implemented a partial smoke-free hospitality industry law. The Netherlands also had several price increases, ran media campaigns about smoking cessation and implemented reimbursement of smoking cessation treatment. Even though we assume that these measures did not exert substantial impact on the warning label-specific measures used in this study, we cannot completely rule out this possibility.

Another limitation is that we examined changes in self-reported warning label responses among continuing smokers and did not assess the impact of pictorial warning labels on quitting behaviour. Additionally, the current study is a population-based survey with a quasi-experimental design. Although this can help us understand the ‘real-world’ impact of pictorial cigarette warning labels, it cannot provide conclusive evidence about causality.

Finally, not all smokers who participated in the baseline survey also completed the follow-up survey. Study participants were older, had a lower educational level, were more often exclusively smoking rolling tobacco, were heavier smokers and were less often planning to quit smoking within 6 months than those who were lost to follow-up or who stopped smoking. Therefore, our results may not be fully generalisable to the population of smokers in the four European countries.

Implications

In 2016, the European directive 2014/40/EU comes into effect, which means that pictorial warning labels will be implemented on 65% of both sides of the cigarette package in the entire European Union. As larger warnings26 and warnings on both sides of the pack6 are more often noticed than smaller warnings on one side of the pack, it is possible that the new EU legislation will increase warning salience compared with the current text-only labels and the current pictorial labels on one side of the pack. A thorough monitoring of the impact of this new legislation is needed in which educational differences are taken into account. Also, our findings from Germany and the Netherlands imply that the warnings need to be changed regularly to prevent wear-out.

It is known from previous studies that cognitive responses and reports of forgoing cigarettes are positively associated with self-reported quit attempts at later time points, both directly27 ,28 and indirectly.29 This implies that the UK warning labels may have a positive impact on quit attempt behaviour. Future studies should explicitly address this question by also including smokers who quit smoking in the analyses and examining effects on behaviour.

We found that warning avoidance increased in France, the UK and Germany. Previous population-based survey studies have found that avoidance strategies do not appear to reduce cessation behaviours.27 ,29 ,30 Future studies should examine whether this applies to smokers from all educational groups.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the current study found that the warning labels implemented in France in 2010 and in the UK in 2008 with pictures on one side of the cigarette package did not succeed in increasing warning salience, but did increase avoidance of warnings. Cognitive responses to the warning labels and forgoing cigarettes because of the warning labels increased only in the UK, and decreased in France. Pictorial warning labels did not seem to increase educational inequalities in self-reported warning label responses among continuing smokers.

What this paper adds

  • The warning labels implemented in France in 2010 and in the UK in 2008 with pictures on one side of the cigarette package did not succeed in increasing warning salience, but did increase avoidance of warnings.

  • Cognitive responses to the warning labels and forgoing cigarettes because of the warning labels increased only in the UK, and decreased in France.

  • Pictorial warning labels on one side of the package did not seem to increase educational inequalities in self-reported warning label responses among continuing smokers.

Acknowledgments

Several members of the ITC Project team at the University of Waterloo have assisted in all stages of conducting the ITC Netherlands surveys, which the authors gratefully acknowledge. In particular, the authors thank Thomas Agar and Anne Quah. This paper is a deliverable within the SILNE Project ‘Tackling socio-economic inequalities in smoking: Learning from natural experiments by time trend analyses and cross-national comparisons’.

References

Footnotes

  • Twitter Follow Gera Nagelhout at @geranagelhout

  • Contributors GEN conducted the statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript. She is the guarantor of the paper. MCW, JFT and H-HY contributed to the design of the paper. UM provided country-specific information for Germany, SCH for UK and RG for France. HdV, AEK, MS, BvdP and GTF contributed to the paper on behalf of the SILNE-ITC Project team. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the data and to the writing of the manuscript. All authors revised the manuscript critically for important intellectual content, and read and approved the final manuscript.

  • Funding The ITC Europe Surveys were supported by grants from Institut nationale du cancer (INCa), Observatoire Français des drogues et toxicomanies (OFDT), and Institut national de prevention et d'education pour la sante (INPES) for ITC France Survey; Cancer Research UK (C312/A3726, C312/A6465, C312/A11039; C312/A11943) for ITC UK Survey; German Federal Ministry of Health, Dieter Mennekes-Umweltstiftung and Germany Cancer Research Center (DKFZ) for ITC Germany Survey; and the Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (ZonMw 70000001 and 121010008) for ITC Netherlands Survey. Geoffrey T Fong is supported by a Senior Investigator Award from the Ontario Institute for Cancer Research and a Prevention Scientist Award from the Canadian Cancer Society Research Institute. The SILNE Project is funded by the European Commission under the FP7-Health-2011 programme (grant agreement 278273).

  • Competing interests None declared.

  • Ethics approval All surveys were cleared for ethics by the Office of Research Ethics of the University of Waterloo, Canada, and by the appropriate institutions in each country.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.