Skip to main content
Log in

Faster P300 latency after smoking in visual but not auditory oddball tasks

  • Original Investigation
  • Published:
Psychopharmacology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In two separate experiments, P300 was recorded from overnight-abstaining smokers before and after smoking. In the first experiment, 32 subjects counted forward by ones and counted backwards by threes upon presentation of a rare tone burst (20%) in a stream of standard tones. There were no changes in P300 amplitude or latency pre- to post-smoking (1.1-mg FTC nicotine-yield cigarette). In the second experiment, 29 subjects completed auditory and visual oddball tasks before smoking, after smoking a low nicotine-yield cigarette (0.05 mg), after smoking a higher nicotine-yield cigarette (1.1-mg), and after smoking a second 1.1-mg cigarette. In the visual oddball task, P300 latency decreased after smoking the first higher-yield cigarette relative to both pre-smoking and post smoking the lower-yield cigarette. This effect was maintained after smoking the second higher-yield cigarette. In the visual task, P300 amplitude increased after smoking the first higher-yield cigarette (from a lower baseline level) in a group of subjects with larger changes in tidal-breath CO but not in a group with smaller changes in CO. There were no effects of smoking on P300 amplitude or latency in the auditory tasks of either the first or second experiment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates T, Pellet O, Stough C, Mangan G (1994) Effects of smoking on simple and choice reaction time. Psychopharmacology 114:365–368

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards JA, Wesnes K, Warburton DM, Gale A (1985) Evidence of more rapid stimulus evaluation following cigarette smoking. Addict Behav 10:113–126

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hasenfratz M, Michel C, Nil R, Battig K (1989) Can smoking increase attention in rapid information processing during noise? Electrocortical, physiological and behavioral effects. Psychopharmacology 98:75–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Houlihan M (1994) P300 and cognitive ability: processing demands, equivocation, and speed of processing during simple cognitive tasks. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Ottawa

  • Kerr JS, Sherwood N, Hindmarch I (1991) Separate and combined effects of the social drugs on psychomotor performance. In: Adlkofer F, Thurau K (eds) Effects of nicotine on biological systems. Birkhauser Verlag, Berlin, pp 521–526

    Google Scholar 

  • Koelega HS (1993) Stimulant drugs and vigilance performance: a review. Psychopharmacology 111:1–16

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kutas M, McCarthy G, Donchin E (1977) Augmenting mental chronometry: the P300 as a measure of stimulus evaluation time. Science 197:792–795

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Le Houezec J, Halliday R, Benowitz N, Callaway E, Naylor H, Herzig K (1994) A low dose of subcutaneous nicotine improves information processing in non-smokers. Psychopharmacology 114:628–634

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCarthy G, Donchin E (1981) A metric for thought: a comparison of P300 latency and reaction time. Science 211:77–79

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michel C, Nil R, Buzzi R, Woodson PP, Battig K (1987) Rapid information processing and concomitant event-related brain potentials in smokers differing in CO absorption. Neuropsychobiology 17:161–168

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norton R, Howard R, Brown K (1991) Nicotine dose-dependent effects of smoking on P300 and mood. Med Sci Res 19:355–356

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS, Robinson JH (1995) Effects of nicotine on cognitive performance in humans. In Snel J (ed) Caffeine, nicotine, and social drinking Effects on task performance information processing and subjective responses. Taylor and Francis, New York (in press)

    Google Scholar 

  • Pritchard WS, Robinson JH, Guy TD (1992) Enhancement of continuous performance task reaction time by smoking in non-deprived smokers. Psychopharmacology 108:437–442

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rusted JM, Warburton DM (1992) Facilitation of memory by post trial administration of nicotine: evidence for an attentional explanation. Psychopharmacology 108:452–455

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood N (1993) Effects of nicotine on human psychomotor performance. Human Psychopharmocol 8:155–184

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood N (1994) Cognitive and psychomotor effects of nicotine and cigarette smoking. In: Ogdon MW, Burton HR, Renfro LW (eds) Recent advances in tobacco science 20:81–105

    Google Scholar 

  • Sherwood N, Kerr JS, Hindmarch I (1992) Psychomotor performance in smokers following single and repeated doses of nicotine gum. Psychopharmacology 108:432–436

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sloan JW, Martin WR, Smith WT (1987) Multiple nicotinic receptors: nicotinic ligands with different specificities. in: Martin WR, Van Loon GR, ET Iwamoto, Davis L (eds) Tobacco smoking and nicotine: a neurobiological approach. Plenum Press, New York, pp 81–100

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelmack RM, Houlihan M, McGarry-Roberts P (1993) Personality, reaction time, and event-related potentials. J Person Soc Psycho 65:399–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K (1987) Nicotine increases mental efficiency: But how? In: Martin WR, Van Loon GR, Iwamoto ET, Davis L (eds) Tobacco smoking and nicotine: a neurobiological approach. Plenum Press, New York, pp 63–80

    Google Scholar 

  • Wesnes K, Warburton DM (1978) The effects of cigarette smoke and nicotine upon human attention. In: Thorton RE (ed) Smoking behavior: physiological and psychological influences. Churchill Livingstone, Edinburgh, pp 131–147

    Google Scholar 

  • West RJ, Jarvis MJ (1986) Effects of nicotine on finger tapping rate in non-smokers. Pharm Bioch & Beh 25:727–731

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Houlihan, M.E., Pritchard, W.S. & Robinson, J.H. Faster P300 latency after smoking in visual but not auditory oddball tasks. Psychopharmacology 123, 231–238 (1996). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02246577

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02246577

Key words

Navigation