Elsevier

Acta Psychologica

Volume 150, July 2014, Pages 153-160
Acta Psychologica

Immediate emotion-enhanced memory dependent on arousal and valence: The role of automatic and controlled processing

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The immediate EEM dependent on valence is associated with controlled processing.

  • The immediate EEM dependent on arousal is moderated by valence.

  • The immediate EEM of arousal for negative stimuli is based on automatic processing.

  • The immediate EEM of arousal for positive stimuli is based on controlled processing.

Abstract

The phenomenon that emotional stimuli are better remembered than neutral ones is called emotion-enhanced memory (EEM). Previous studies have shown that both valence and arousal of stimuli contributed to EEM. Kensinger and Corkin (2004) proposed that the EEM dependent on arousal was associated with automatic encoding processes, whereas the EEM dependent on valence was associated with controlled encoding processes. Their experiment with negative words provided some evidence for this associative pattern. However, it is unclear whether the observed association that occurred with negative emotional stimuli could be replicated with positive emotional stimuli. To further examine this issue, two experiments were conducted to investigate the immediate EEM of emotional words in three different attention conditions using a divided attention (DA) paradigm. Results indicated that the immediate EEM dependent on valence was associated with controlled processing, while the immediate EEM dependent on arousal was not always associated with automatic processing. The immediate EEM dependent on arousal for negative stimuli was associated with automatic processing, whereas the immediate EEM dependent on arousal for positive stimuli was associated with controlled processing. Therefore, the immediate EEM dependent on arousal, whether it is associated with automatic or controlled processing, is moderated by the valence of stimuli.

Introduction

Emotional stimuli are more easily remembered than neutral ones. This phenomenon is called emotion-enhanced memory (EEM) (LaBar and Cabeza, 2006, Phelps et al., 1997, Talmi et al., 2007). EEM has been observed in recognition, free recall and cued recall performances using various emotional stimuli types, from words and pictures to film clips (Bradley et al., 1992, Buchanan and Adolphs, 2002, Burton et al., 2004, Denburg et al., 2003, Doerksen and Shimamura, 2001, Döhnel et al., 2008, Kensinger et al., 2002, MacKay et al., 2004, Richardson et al., 2004, Talmi et al., 2007). Existing studies have shown that EEM is driven by both the valence of stimuli and the arousal intensity of induced emotion (Kensinger, 2004, Kensinger and Corkin, 2003, LaBar, 2007). For instance, studies found that memory for highly arousing stimuli, especially highly arousing negative stimuli, is better than that for neutral ones. This suggests that arousal is a critical factor in enhancing memory (Anderson et al., 2006, Canli et al., 2002, Dolcos and Denkova, 2008, Kensinger et al., 2007, Kensinger and Schacter, 2007, Mather and Sutherland, 2011, McGaugh, 2004). Other studies found that memory for negative or positive emotional stimuli is better than for neutral stimuli, even when the arousal intensity of such stimuli is low. This suggests that valence is another important factor in boosting memory performance (Berntsen, 2002, Kensinger, 2009, Kensinger and Corkin, 2003, Kensinger and Corkin, 2004, Kensinger and Schacter, 2008, Kensinger et al., 2002, LaBar and Cabeza, 2006, Mickley and Kensinger, 2008, Ochsner, 2000, Talmi et al., 2007, Van Steenbergen et al., 2010).

Neuroimaging studies have shown that arousal and valence influence emotional memory via modulation of distinct neural mechanisms (Kensinger and Corkin, 2004, Kensinger and Schacter, 2005, Murty et al., 2010, Steinmetz and Kensinger, 2009). For arousing stimuli, the amygdala plays a critical role in EEM by modulating encoding processes, especially for negative arousing stimuli (Anderson et al., 2006, Kensinger and Schacter, 2005, Kensinger and Schacter, 2006, Sharot et al., 2004). The level of activation in the amygdala and hippocampus is proportional to the likelihood that emotionally arousing stimuli are remembered (Dolcos et al., 2004, Kensinger and Corkin, 2004, LaBar and Cabeza, 2006, Phelps, 2004). In contrast, memory for negative nonarousing stimuli is boosted due to differential engagement of the prefrontal (PFC) network (Anderson et al., 2006, Kensinger and Corkin, 2004, Kensinger and Schacter, 2005), especially the right PFC (Davidson & Irwin, 1999).

Based on the findings of these neuroimaging studies, Kensinger and Corkin (2004) proposed that arousal and valence might play different roles in encoding processes contributing to the EEM via distinct neural routes. The EEM of a negative arousing stimulus is mediated by the amygdala-hippocampal network, which is associated with relatively automatic encoding processes, whereas, the EEM of a negative nonarousing stimulus is supported by a PFC-hippocampal network, which is associated with controlled encoding processes. Behavioral data using negative words by Kensinger and Corkin (2004) supported this associative pattern. However, the distinct neural mechanisms do not necessarily correspond with automatic and controlled processing in behavioral performance because the EEM of arousing stimuli does not solely depend on activation in the amygdala, and the EEM of nonarousing stimuli does not solely depend on prefrontal activation. Moreover, Kensinger and Corkin's study (2004) did not examine how automatic and controlled processing would contribute to the EEM dependent on arousal and valence in a positive stimulus condition. Therefore, more evidence is needed to clarify whether or not the EEM dependent on arousal is associated with automatic processing and whether or not the EEM dependent on valence is associated with controlled processing.

Beyond the research just discussed, related studies have shown that the cognitive processing and neural mechanisms of negative stimuli are different from those of positive stimuli (Christianson, 1992, Clark-Foos and Marsh, 2008, Kern et al., 2005, Steinmetz et al., 2010, Talmi et al., 2007). For instance, Steinmetz et al. (2010) found that, in contrast to processing of negative stimuli, the amygdala was activated when processing positive low-arousing stimuli and amygdala efferents weakened as arousal increased for positive stimuli. Electrophysiological evidence has also shown that threat-related stimuli may elicit an early enhanced encoding (amplitude enhancement on frontal sites), and this early effect might be processed unconsciously relative to happy and neutral stimuli (Eimer et al., 2008, Righi et al., 2012). Talmi et al. (2007) argued that attention mediation can completely account for the effect of positive emotion on memory, but the influence of negative emotion on memory is not significantly mediated via attention. Moreover, from an evolutionary perspective, the biological information attached to positive and negative stimuli is different from each other (Bradley and Lang, 2007, Lang, 2010). Prior research on the relation between emotional valence and goals has suggested that people feel negative emotion when goals are threatened and feel positive emotion when goal attainment is anticipated or has been achieved (Levine & Edelstein, 2009). The information relevant to uncompleted goals tends to be well remembered whereas information relevant to completed goals tends to be forgotten (Förster, Liberman, & Higgins, 2005). Taken together, the growing body of evidence indicates that the cognitive processing of negative stimuli differs from that of positive stimuli. Accordingly, the associative pattern between the EEM dependent on arousal or valence and the automatic or controlled processing for negative emotional stimuli might be different from that for positive emotional stimuli.

Recently, the contributions of automatic and controlled processing to the EEM have been reported in other studies. For example, Talmi et al., 2007, Talmi and McGarry, 2012, distinguished between immediate and delayed EEM (referring separately to memory tested immediately, or at relatively short delays and at long delays, such as hours or even days, as each is believed to depend on different processes). They found that immediately positive EEM was completely mediated by secondary-task performance (controlled attention) during encoding, but immediately negative EEM was not. Kern et al. (2005) reported that in contrast to full attention, divided attention would adversely impact neutral and positive arousing words more than negative words. Chainay, Michael, Vert-pré, Landré, and Plasson (2012) reported that the effect of emotion on immediate memory enhancement may depend on the intentionality to encode for positive pictures. Pottage and Schaefer (2012) found that visual attention and automatic preattentive processing both play important roles in the immediate EEM for negative pictures. Maddox, Naveh-Benjamin, Old, and Kilb (2012) found that automatic processing contributes to immediate EEM for positive words as well as for negative words. Talmi et al. (2013) found that food images had parallel effects on attention and memory for hungry participants, which implies that superior memory for food images is not mediated by the attention devoted to them. However, the studies mentioned above simply investigated the effect of emotionality on immediate EEM, and did not distinguish the EEM effects dependent on arousal and valence. Therefore, it is difficult to infer from them which processes, automatic or controlled, contributed to the immediate EEM dependent on arousal and valence of stimuli.

The present study examined the contributions of automatic and controlled processing to the immediate EEM dependent on arousal and valence, especially for positive emotional stimuli. The negative/positive arousing, negative/positive nonarousing, and neutral Chinese words were employed in the experiment. To control the residual processing resources involved in the immediate EEM, three different attention conditions which included FA (full attention), easy DA (divided attention with an easy concurrent secondary task) and difficult DA (divided attention with a difficult concurrent secondary task) were designed. The effect of valence was investigated by comparing recognition memories for nonarousing and neutral words. Similarly, the effect of arousal was investigated by comparing recognition memories for arousing and nonarousing words.

Section snippets

Experiment 1: the role of automatic and controlled processing in the immediate EEM of negative words

To further test the particular contributions of automatic and controlled processing to the immediate EEM dependent on arousal and valence for negative stimuli, Experiment 1 replicated Kensinger and Corkin's study (2004) with Chinese words. In Experiment 1, negative arousing, negative nonarousing, and neutral Chinese words were used as experimental materials. Participants were first asked to learn negative arousing, negative nonarousing, and neutral Chinese words under three different attention

Experiment 2: the role of automatic and controlled processing in the immediate EEM of positive words

As noted previously, growing evidence shows that the cognitive processing of positive stimuli is different from that of negative stimuli. Thus, we speculated that, for positive stimuli, the associative pattern between the EEM dependent on arousal or valence and automatic or controlled processing is different from that of negative stimuli. To further examine this issue, in Experiment 2, positive arousing, positive nonarousing, and neutral Chinese words were used as experimental materials.

General discussion

The two experiments clearly show that the immediate EEM dependent on arousal and valence was observed in the FA condition, which was in line with previous studies (Anderson et al., 2006, Canli et al., 2000, Canli et al., 2002, Kensinger and Corkin, 2003, Kensinger and Schacter, 2007, Kensinger and Schacter, 2008, Kensinger et al., 2002, LaBar and Cabeza, 2006, Ochsner, 2000, Talmi et al., 2007). In particular, the present study found that a concurrent secondary task during encoding adversely

References (66)

  • E.A. Phelps et al.

    Memory for emotional words following unilateral temporal lobectomy

    Brain and Cognition

    (1997)
  • S. Righi et al.

    Fearful expressions enhance recognition memory: Electrophysiological evidence

    Acta Psychologica

    (2012)
  • D. Talmi et al.

    Accounting for immediate emotional memory enhancement

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2012)
  • A.P. Yonelinas

    The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2002)
  • A.K. Anderson et al.

    Emotional memories are not all created equal: Evidence for selective memory enhancement

    Learning & Memory

    (2006)
  • D. Berntsen

    Tunnel memories for autobiographical events: Central details are remembered more frequently from shocking than from happy experiences

    Memory & Cognition

    (2002)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1992)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Emotion and motivation

  • T.W. Buchanan et al.

    The role of the human amygdala in emotional modulation of long-term declarative memory

  • L. Burton et al.

    Affective tasks elicit material-specific memory effects in temporal lobectomy patients

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • T. Canli et al.

    Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2002)
  • T. Canli et al.

    Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience

    The Journal of Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • H. Chainay et al.

    Emotional enhancement of immediate memory: Positive pictorial stimuli are better recognized than neutral or negative pictorial stimuli

    Advances in Cognitive Psychology

    (2012)
  • S.A. Christianson

    Emotional stress and eyewitness memory: A critical review

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1992)
  • S.A. Christianson et al.

    Memory and emotional consistency: The MS Estonia ferry disaster

    Memory

    (1999)
  • A. Clark-Foos et al.

    Recognition memory for valenced and arousing materials under conditions of divided attention

    Memory

    (2008)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    The effects of attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1996)
  • A. D'Argembeau et al.

    Facial expressions of emotion influence memory for facial identity in an automatic way

    Emotion

    (2007)
  • N.L. Denburg et al.

    Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal older persons

    Emotion

    (2003)
  • S. Doerksen et al.

    Source memory enhancement for emotional words

    Emotion

    (2001)
  • F. Dolcos et al.

    Neural correlates of encoding emotional memories: A review of functional neuroimaging evidence

    Cell Science Review

    (2008)
  • M. Eimer et al.

    Links between rapid ERP responses to fearful faces and conscious awareness

    Journal of Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • B.L. Fredrickson

    Neural correlates of self-reflection

    Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences

    (2004)
  • Cited by (24)

    • The effects of arousal and attention on emotional false memory formation

      2019, Journal of Memory and Language
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, this was not the case for high arousing negative associations, where false memories for critical lures persisted even with fast presentation time. Although not directly testing such theory, our results provide further support that emotion modulates (false) memory through an automatic route primarily consisting of the amygdala and brain regions related to memory, which are less dependent on attentional resources (Clark-Foos & Marsh, 2008; Kang et al., 2014; Kern, Libkuman, Otani, & Holmes, 2005; Talmi, Schimmack et al., 2007). Under conditions of limited attentional resources, high arousing negative stimuli that are automatically processed should then allow for the extraction of gist or the activation of associative connections when encoding negative high arousing DRM lists (Knott et al., 2018).

    • Dissociation of immediate and delayed effects of emotional arousal on episodic memory

      2018, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Memory undoubtedly benefits from the attention emotionally-arousing stimuli capture during encoding. Enhanced attention to such stimuli, as reflected, for instance, in more fixations, results in deeper processing during encoding and better immediate memory (Bradley, Houbova, Miccoli, Costa, & Lang, 2011; Kang, Wang, Surina, & Lü, 2014; Miendlarzewska, van Elswijk, Cannistraci, & van Ee, 2013; Sharot & Phelps, 2004; Talmi, Anderson, Riggs, Caplan, & Moscovitch, 2008). Moreover, enhanced attention to emotional stimuli during encoding is also consistent with the more frequent experience of recollection during the recognition of emotional items (Kensinger, Clarke, & Corkin, 2003; Kensinger & Corkin, 2003).

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported by the National Science Foundation Grant (30970912) awarded to Zhenhong Wang.

    View full text